Bitter Taste Rejection: A Marker for Dietary Trophic Levels |
Proposition A:However, the claim that is promoted in raw circles is the logical converse,
Natural toxin implies bitter taste.
Proposition B:This section will focus on
Bitter taste implies natural toxin.
Lack of proof for the claim that bitter implies toxic. Glendinning [1994] defines the bitter rejection response as the aversive reaction that occurs when one eats bitter foods. He then asks whether the claim that bitter implies toxic is valid, and reports that
The bitter rejection response has not been evaluated rigorously in terms of its value in avoiding poisons. For the bitter rejection response to be an effective poison detector, there should be a predictable relationship between the threshold concentration for bitterness and that for toxicity. If the bitter threshold is substantially higher or lower than the toxicity threshold for many compounds, the effectiveness of the bitter rejection response as a poison detector would be uncertain. To the author's knowledge, no investigator has explicitly tested for such a relationship across a series of bitter compounds.Evidence that the claim "bitter implies toxic" is false. The hypothesis is then presented that perhaps the bitter taste threshold co-varies with the level of toxicity of the bitter compounds, i.e., that proposition B above (the fruitarian/
Taken together, the above studies indicate that bitter taste sensitivity does not accurately mirror the body's systemic reactivity to naturally ocurring or synthetic [toxic] compounds in several species of mammal.Fallacious "proofs" that "bitter implies toxic" as cited in raw circles. The reality that there is no legitimate scientific proof for the fruitarian/
Similarly, those animals whose diet almost never includes the bitter taste, i.e., carnivores, would rarely encounter the bitter taste and have a low tolerance for it, hence have very low bitter rejection thresholds. Omnivores would fall in between the two extremes in bitter taste tolerance. Glendinning [1994] hypothesizes that the bitter rejection thresholds would follow the order, from lowest to highest threshold
TROPHIC GROUP |
QHCL Taste |
|
Carnivores |
2.1 x 10-5 M |
|
Humans |
3.0 x 10-5 M |
|
Omnivores |
3.0 x 10-4 M |
|
Herbivores |
Grazers |
6.7 x 10-4 M |
Browsers |
3.0 x 10-3 M |
The significance of Glendinning [1994] is that the study provides an actual analysis of bitter taste tolerance data for a wide variety of species, and reaches a result which suggests, or points to the possibility based on the data obtained (which is of course not proof by itself) that humans may be carnivores/
No doubt certain fruitarian extremists will react to the above and claim that humans reject the bitter taste because our natural food is (nearly exclusively) sweet fruit. However, as discussed in previous sections, there is no legitimate scientific evidence for the claim that humans evolved as fruitarians (whether strictly so or not).
Additionally, many wild fruits are quite bitter, and food shortages or competition for food would likely make the consumption of bitter fruit necessary at times--
GO TO NEXT PART OF ARTICLE
Return to beginning of article
Back to Research-Based Appraisals of Alternative Diet Lore
(Insulin Resistance and The Carnivore Connection Hypothesis)
SEE TABLE OF CONTENTS FOR:
PART 1 PART 2 PART 3 PART 4 PART 5 PART 6 PART 7 PART 8 PART 9
GO TO PART 1 - Brief Overview: What is the Relevance of Comparative Anatomical and Physiological "Proofs"?
GO TO PART 2 - Looking at Ape Diets: Myths, Realities, and Rationalizations
GO TO PART 3 - The Fossil-Record Evidence about
GO TO PART 4 - Intelligence, Evolution of the Human Brain,
GO TO PART 5 - Limitations on Comparative Dietary Proofs
GO TO PART 6 - What Comparative Anatomy Does and Doesn't Tell Us about
GO TO PART 7 - Insights about Human Nutrition & Digestion from Comparative Physiology
GO TO PART 8 - Further Issues in the Debate over Omnivorous vs. Vegetarian Diets
GO TO PART 9 - Conclusions: The End, or The Beginning of a New Approach to